Created
June 17, 2012 16:54
-
-
Save grero/2945056 to your computer and use it in GitHub Desktop.
This file contains bidirectional Unicode text that may be interpreted or compiled differently than what appears below. To review, open the file in an editor that reveals hidden Unicode characters.
Learn more about bidirectional Unicode characters
Fisher and shannon information in finite neural populations. | |
Stuart Yarrow, Edward Challis, and Peggy Seriès. | |
The precision of the neural code is commonly investigated using two | |
families of statistical measures: Shannon mutual information and | |
derived quantities when investigating very small populations of | |
neurons and Fisher information when studying large populations. These | |
statistical tools are no longer the preserve of theorists and are | |
being applied by experimental research groups in the analysis of | |
empirical data. Although the relationship between | |
information-theoretic and Fisher-based measures in the limit of | |
infinite populations is relatively well understood, how these measures | |
compare in finite-size populations has not yet been systematically | |
explored. We aim to close this gap. We are particularly interested in | |
understanding which stimuli are best encoded by a given neuron within | |
a population and how this depends on the chosen measure. We use a | |
novel Monte Carlo approach to compute a stimulus-specific | |
decomposition of the mutual information (the SSI) for populations of | |
up to 256 neurons and show that Fisher information can be used to | |
accurately estimate both mutual information and SSI for populations of | |
the order of 100 neurons, even in the presence of biologically | |
realistic variability, noise correlations, and experimentally relevant | |
integration times. According to both measures, the stimuli that are | |
best encoded are those falling at the flanks of the neuron's tuning | |
curve. In populations of fewer than around 50 neurons, however, Fisher | |
information can be misleading. | |
Neural Comput, 2012 vol. 24 (7) pp. 1740-1780 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=22428594&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comment: This paper mainly highlights a different method of computing | |
stimulus relevant information, using the fisher information, which is | |
related to the minimum variance achievable for any estimator. To | |
compute it, one has to postulate a model for estimating the stimulus | |
from the data, though, and then compute the covariance matrix of that | |
estimate across all neurons. The reciprocal of the determinant of that | |
matrix is then an estimate of the fisher information | |
##################################################################################### | |
Sparse low-order interaction network underlies a highly correlated and | |
learnable neural population code. | |
Elad Ganmor, Ronen Segev, and Elad Schneidman. | |
Information is carried in the brain by the joint activity patterns of | |
large groups of neurons. Understanding the structure and function of | |
population neural codes is challenging because of the exponential | |
number of possible activity patterns and dependencies among neurons. | |
We report here that for groups of ~100 retinal neurons responding to | |
natural stimuli, pairwise-based models, which were highly accurate for | |
small networks, are no longer sufficient. We show that because of the | |
sparse nature of the neural code, the higher-order interactions can be | |
easily learned using a novel model and that a very sparse low-order | |
interaction network underlies the code of large populations of | |
neurons. Additionally, we show that the interaction network is | |
organized in a hierarchical and modular manner, which hints at | |
scalability. Our results suggest that learnability may be a key | |
feature of the neural code. | |
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2011 vol. 108 (23) | |
pp. 9679-9684 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=21602497&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comment: They use a variant of the maximum entropy model that does not | |
directly compute all the possible pairwise expectation values, but | |
rather uses Monte Carlo methods to minimize minimize the distance | |
between the true and the empirical distribution. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Correlations, feature-binding and population coding in primary visual cortex. | |
Huw D R Golledge, Stefano Panzeri, Fashan Zheng, Gianni Pola, Jack W | |
Scannell, Dimitrios V Giannikopoulos, Roger J Mason, Martin J Tovée, | |
and Malcolm P Young. | |
To test the hypothesis that correlated neuronal activity serves as the | |
neuronal code for visual feature binding, we applied information | |
theory techniques to multiunit activity recorded from pairs of V1 | |
recording sites in anaesthetised cats while presenting either single | |
or separate bar stimuli. We quantified the roles of firing rates of | |
individual channels and of cross-correlations between recording sites | |
in encoding of visual information. Between 89 and 96% of the | |
information was carried by firing rates; correlations contributed | |
4-11% extra information. The distribution across the population of | |
either correlation strength or correlation information did not co-vary | |
systematically with changes in perception predicted by Gestalt | |
psychology. These results suggest that firing rates, rather than | |
correlations, are the main element of the population code for feature | |
binding in primary visual cortex. | |
Neuroreport, 2003 vol. 14 (7) pp. 1045-1050 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=12802200&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comment: This is a straight forward computation of information encoded | |
by pairs of neurons in primary visual cortex. Perhaps more relevant | |
for the shufflesync paper. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Information processing with population codes. | |
A POUGET, P Dayan, and R Zemel. | |
Information is encoded in the brain by populations or clusters of | |
cells, rather than by single cells. This encoding strategy is known as | |
population coding. Here we review the standard use of population codes | |
for encoding and decoding information, and consider how population | |
codes can be used to support neural computations such as noise removal | |
and nonlinear mapping. More radical ideas about how population codes | |
may directly represent information about stimulus uncertainty are also | |
discussed. | |
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2000 vol. 1 (2) pp. 125-132 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=11252775&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comment: A nice review paper on the use of information theory for | |
population encoding and decoding. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Neuronal population coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the | |
primate posterior parietal cortex. | |
Oana Tudusciuc and Andreas Nieder. | |
Quantitative knowledge guides vital decisions in the life of animals | |
and humans alike. The posterior parietal cortex in primates has been | |
implicated in representing abstract quantity, both continuous (extent) | |
and discrete (number of items), supporting the idea of a putative | |
generalized magnitude system in this brain area. Whether or not single | |
neurons encode different types of quantity, or how quantitative | |
information is represented in the neuronal responses, however, is | |
unknown. We show that length and numerosity are encoded by | |
functionally overlapping groups of parietal neurons. Using a | |
statistical classifier, we found that the activity of populations of | |
quantity-selective neurons contained accurate information about | |
continuous and discrete quantity. Unexpectedly, even neurons that were | |
nonselective according to classical spike-count measures conveyed | |
robust categorical information that predicted the monkeys' quantity | |
judgments. Thus, different information-carrying processes of partly | |
intermingled neuronal networks in the parietal lobe seem to encode | |
various forms of abstract quantity. | |
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2007 vol. 104 (36) pp. 14513-14518 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=17724337&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comment: Uses a statistical decoder to quantify the amount of | |
information carried by populations of neurons | |
##################################################################################### | |
Dynamic population coding of category information in inferior temporal | |
and prefrontal cortex. | |
Ethan M Meyers, David J Freedman, Gabriel Kreiman, Earl K Miller, and | |
Tomaso Poggio. | |
Most electrophysiology studies analyze the activity of each neuron | |
separately. While such studies have given much insight into properties | |
of the visual system, they have also potentially overlooked important | |
aspects of information coded in changing patterns of activity that are | |
distributed over larger populations of neurons. In this work, we apply | |
a population decoding method to better estimate what information is | |
available in neuronal ensembles and how this information is coded in | |
dynamic patterns of neural activity in data recorded from inferior | |
temporal cortex (ITC) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) as macaque monkeys | |
engaged in a delayed match-to-category task. Analyses of activity | |
patterns in ITC and PFC revealed that both areas contain "abstract" | |
category information (i.e., category information that is not directly | |
correlated with properties of the stimuli); however, in general, PFC | |
has more task-relevant information, and ITC has more detailed visual | |
information. Analyses examining how information coded in these areas | |
show that almost all category information is available in a small | |
fraction of the neurons in the population. Most remarkably, our | |
results also show that category information is coded by a | |
nonstationary pattern of activity that changes over the course of a | |
trial with individual neurons containing information on much shorter | |
time scales than the population as a whole. | |
Journal of Neurophysiology, 2008 vol. 100 (3) pp. 1407-1419 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=18562555&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comment: Uses a linear classifier to find a lower bound on the amount | |
of knowledge encoded by populations of neurons. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Decoding the activity of neuronal populations in macaque primary visual cortex. | |
Arnulf B A Graf, Adam Kohn, Mehrdad Jazayeri, and J Anthony Movshon. | |
Visual function depends on the accuracy of signals carried by visual | |
cortical neurons. Combining information across neurons should improve | |
this accuracy because single neuron activity is variable. We examined | |
the reliability of information inferred | |
from populations of simultaneously recorded neurons in macaque primary | |
visual cortex. We considered a decoding framework that computes the | |
likelihood of visual stimuli from a pattern of population activity by | |
linearly combining neuronal responses | |
and tested this framework for orientation estimation and | |
discrimination. We derived a simple parametric decoder assuming | |
neuronal independence and a more sophisticated empirical decoder that | |
learned the structure of the measured neuronal response distributions, | |
including their correlated variability. The empirical decoder used the | |
structure of these response distributions to perform better than its | |
parametric variant, indicating that their structure contains critical | |
information for sensory decoding. | |
These results show how neuronal responses can best be used to inform | |
perceptual decision-making. | |
Nat. Neurosci., 2011 vol. 14 (2) pp. 239-245 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nn.2733 | |
##################################################################################### | |
Redundancy in the Population Code of the Retina. | |
Jason L Puchalla, Elad Schneidman, Robert A Harris, and Michael J Berry. | |
We have explored the manner in which the population of retinal | |
ganglion cells collectively represent the vi- sual world. Ganglion | |
cells in the salamander were re- corded simultaneously with a | |
multielectrode array during stimulation with both artificial and | |
natural vi- sual stimuli, and the mutual information that single cells | |
and pairs of cells conveyed about the stimulus was estimated. We found | |
significant redundancy be- tween cells spaced as far as 500 m apart. | |
When we used standard methods for defining functional types, only | |
ON-type and OFF-type cells emerged as truly independent information | |
channels. Although the av- erage redundancy between nearby cell pairs | |
was moderate, each ganglion cell shared information with many | |
neighbors, so that visual information was repre- sented 10-fold within | |
the ganglion cell population. This high degree of retinal redundancy | |
suggests that design principles beyond coding efficiency may be | |
important at the population level. | |
Neuron, 2005 vol. 46 (3) pp. 493-504 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627305003119 | |
Comment: This paper only does pair-wise information to look at redundancy. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Excess synchrony in motor cortical neurons provides redundant | |
direction information with that from coarse temporal measures. | |
M W Oram, N G Hatsopoulos, B J Richmond, and J P Donoghue. | |
Previous studies have shown that measures of fine temporal | |
correlation, such as synchronous spikes, across responses of motor | |
cortical neurons carries more directional information than that | |
predicted from statistically independent neurons. It is also known, | |
however, that the coarse temporal measures of responses, such as spike | |
count, are not independent. We therefore examined whether the | |
information carried by coincident firing was related to that of | |
coarsely defined spike counts and their correlation. Synchronous | |
spikes were counted in the responses from 94 pairs of simultaneously | |
recorded neurons in primary motor cortex (MI) while monkeys performed | |
arm movement tasks. Direct measurement of the movement-related | |
information indicated that the coincident spikes (1- to 5-ms | |
precision) carry approximately 10% of the information carried by a | |
code of the two spike counts. Inclusion of the numbers of synchronous | |
spikes did not add information to that available from the spike counts | |
and their coarse temporal correlation. To assess the significance of | |
the numbers of coincident spikes, we extended the stochastic spike | |
count matched (SCM) model to include correlations between spike counts | |
of the individual neural responses and slow temporal dependencies | |
within neural responses (approximately 30 Hz bandwidth). The extended | |
SCM model underestimated the numbers of synchronous spikes. Therefore | |
as with previous studies, we found that there were more synchronous | |
spikes in the neural data than could be accounted for by this | |
stochastic model. However, the SCM model accounts for most (R(2) = | |
0.93 +/- 0.05, mean +/- SE) of the differences in the observed number | |
of synchronous spikes to different directions of arm movement, | |
indicating that synchronous spiking is directly related to spike | |
counts and their broad correlation. Further, this model supports the | |
information theoretic analysis that the synchronous spikes do not | |
provide directional information beyond that available from the firing | |
rates of the same pool of directionally tuned MI neurons. These | |
results show that detection of precisely timed spike patterns above | |
chance levels does not imply that those spike patterns carry | |
information unavailable from coarser population codes but leaves open | |
the possibility that excess synchrony carries other forms of | |
information or serves other roles in cortical information processing | |
not studied here. | |
Journal of Neurophysiology, 2001 vol. 86 (4) pp. 1700-1716 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=11600633&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
##################################################################################### | |
Population coding of stimulus location in rat somatosensory cortex. | |
R S Petersen, S Panzeri, and M E Diamond. | |
This study explores the nature of population coding in sensory cortex | |
by applying information theoretic analyses to neuron pairs recorded | |
simultaneously from rat barrel cortex. We quantified the roles of | |
individual spikes and spike patterns in encoding whisker stimulus | |
location. 82%-85% of the total information was contained in the timing | |
of individual spikes: first spike time was particularly crucial. Spike | |
patterns within neurons accounted for the remaining 15%-18%. Neuron | |
pairs located in the same barrel column coded redundantly, whereas | |
pairs in neighboring barrel columns coded independently. The barrel | |
cortical population code for stimulus location appears to be the time | |
of single neurons' first poststimulus spikes-a fast, robust coding | |
mechanism that does not rely on "synergy" in crossneuronal spike | |
patterns. | |
Neuron, 2001 vol. 32 (3) pp. 503-514 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=11709160&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
##################################################################################### | |
The structure of large-scale synchronized firing in primate retina. | |
Jonathon Shlens, Greg D Field, Jeffrey L Gauthier, Martin Greschner, | |
Alexander Sher, Alan M Litke, and E J Chichilnisky. | |
Synchronized firing among neurons has been proposed to constitute an | |
elementary aspect of the neural code in sensory and motor systems. | |
However, it remains unclear how synchronized firing affects the | |
large-scale patterns of activity and redundancy of visual signals in a | |
complete population of neurons. We recorded simultaneously from | |
hundreds of retinal ganglion cells in primate retina, and examined | |
synchronized firing in completely sampled populations of approximately | |
50-100 ON-parasol cells, which form a major projection to the | |
magnocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Synchronized | |
firing in pairs of cells was a subset of a much larger pattern of | |
activity that exhibited local, isotropic spatial properties. However, | |
a simple model based solely on interactions between adjacent cells | |
reproduced 99% of the spatial structure and scale of synchronized | |
firing. No more than 20% of the variability in firing of an individual | |
cell was predictable from the activity of its neighbors. These results | |
held both for spontaneous firing and in the presence of independent | |
visual modulation of the firing of each cell. In sum, large-scale | |
synchronized firing in the entire population of ON-parasol cells | |
appears to reflect simple neighbor interactions, rather than a unique | |
visual signal or a highly redundant coding scheme. | |
Journal of Neuroscience, 2009 vol. 29 (15) pp. 5022-5031 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=19369571&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
comment: also uses a maximum entropy model to characterize the joint | |
activity of pairs of neurons | |
##################################################################################### | |
Spatio-temporal correlations and visual signalling in a complete | |
neuronal population. | |
Jonathan W Pillow, Jonathon Shlens, Liam Paninski, Alexander Sher, | |
Alan M Litke, E J Chichilnisky, and Eero P Simoncelli. | |
Statistical dependencies in the responses of sensory neurons govern | |
both the amount of stimulus information conveyed and the means by | |
which downstream neurons can extract it. Although a variety of | |
measurements indicate the existence of such dependencies, their origin | |
and importance for neural coding are poorly understood. Here we | |
analyse the functional significance of correlated firing in a complete | |
population of macaque parasol retinal ganglion cells using a model of | |
multi-neuron spike responses. The model, with parameters fit directly | |
to physiological data, simultaneously captures both the stimulus | |
dependence and detailed spatio-temporal correlations in population | |
responses, and provides two insights into the structure of the neural | |
code. First, neural encoding at the population level is less noisy | |
than one would expect from the variability of individual neurons: | |
spike times are more precise, and can be predicted more accurately | |
when the spiking of neighbouring neurons is taken into account. | |
Second, correlations provide additional sensory information: optimal, | |
model-based decoding that exploits the response correlation structure | |
extracts 20% more information about the visual scene than decoding | |
under the assumption of independence, and preserves 40% more visual | |
information than optimal linear decoding. This model-based approach | |
reveals the role of correlated activity in the retinal coding of | |
visual stimuli, and provides a general framework for understanding the | |
importance of correlated activity in populations of neurons. | |
Nature, 2008 vol. 454 (7207) pp. 995-999 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nature07140 | |
Comment: uses a generalized linear model to show that decoding using | |
correlations gives more information than ignoring them. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Neural correlations, population coding and computation. | |
Bruno B Averbeck, Peter E Latham, and Alexandre Pouget. | |
How the brain encodes information in population activity, and how it | |
combines and manipulates that activity as it carries out computations, | |
are questions that lie at the heart of systems neuroscience. During | |
the past decade, with the advent of multi-electrode recording and | |
improved theoretical models, these questions have begun to yield | |
answers. However, a complete understanding of neuronal variability, | |
and, in particular, how it affects population codes, is missing. This | |
is because variability in the brain is typically correlated, and | |
although the exact effects of these correlations are not known, it is | |
known that they can be large. Here, we review studies that address the | |
interaction between neuronal noise and population codes, and discuss | |
their implications for population coding in general. | |
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2006 vol. 7 (5) pp. 358-366 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nrn1888 | |
Comment: Another nice review of population coding | |
##################################################################################### | |
Population coding of orientation in the visual cortex of alert | |
cats--an information theoretic analysis. | |
Christoph Kayser and Peter König. | |
We studied the encoding of stimulus orientation in the visual cortex | |
of alert animals using information theory methods. Based on a | |
labeled-line code, the encoding of orientation was mostly synergistic | |
and only few pairs coded redundant. The synergy contributed about 20% | |
of the information and was strongest for sites with distinct tuning | |
curves. A recently proposed decomposition of synergy revealed that | |
redundancy introduced by common tuning preferences is more than just | |
compensated by noise correlations which mostly contributed | |
synergistically. Based on a pooled response code, the contribution of | |
noise correlations diminished resulting in a severe information loss. | |
Thus, to operate economically, cortical neurons should either employ a | |
labeled-line code or, if using pooled responses, be highly selective | |
in choosing afferents. | |
Neuroreport, 2004 vol. 15 (18) pp. 2761-2764 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&id=15597049&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks | |
Comments: Another paper computing pair-wise information using firing rates. | |
##################################################################################### | |
Information Transmission and Detection Thresholds in the Vestibular | |
Nuclei: Single Neurons versus Population Encoding. | |
C Massot, M J Chacron, and K E Cullen. | |
Under- standing how sensory neurons transmit information about | |
relevant stimuli remains a major goal in neuroscience. Of particular | |
relevance are the roles of neural variability and spike timing in | |
neural coding. Peripheral vestibular afferents display differential | |
variability that is correlated with the importance of spike timing; | |
regular afferents display little variability and use a timing code to | |
transmit information about sensory input. Irregular afferents, | |
conversely, display greater variability and instead use a rate code. | |
We studied how central neurons within the vestibular nuclei integrate | |
information from both afferent classes by recording from a group of | |
neurons termed vestib- ular only (VO) that are known to make | |
contributions to vestibulospi- nal reflexes and project to | |
higher-order centers. We found that, although individual central | |
neurons had sensitivities that were greater than or equal to those of | |
individual afferents, they transmitted less information. In addition, | |
their velocity detection thresholds were significantly greater than | |
those of individual afferents. This is because VO neurons display | |
greater variability, which is detrimental to infor- mation | |
transmission and signal detection. Combining activities from multiple | |
VO neurons increased information transmission. However, the | |
information rates were still much lower than those of equivalent | |
afferent populations. Furthermore, combining responses from multi- ple | |
VO neurons led to lower velocity detection threshold values | |
approaching those measured from behavior (2.5 vs. 0.5–1°/s). Our | |
results suggest that the detailed time course of vestibular stimuli | |
encoded by afferents is not transmitted by VO neurons. Instead, they | |
suggest that higher vestibular pathways must integrate information | |
from central vestibular neuron populations to give rise to | |
behaviorally observed detection thresholds. | |
Journal of Neurophysiology, 2011 vol. 105 pp. 1798-1814 | |
http://libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/login?url=http://jn.physiology.org/cgi/doi/10.1152/jn.00910.2010 | |
Comments: They assume that neurons are independent, and compute the | |
joint information by summing the information of individual neurons. |
Sign up for free
to join this conversation on GitHub.
Already have an account?
Sign in to comment